Документ взят из кэша поисковой машины. Адрес оригинального документа : http://www.philol.msu.ru/~otipl/new/mscl/2008/abstracts/brykina.pdf
Дата изменения: Fri Mar 21 13:05:06 2008
Дата индексирования: Thu Apr 8 09:35:46 2010
Кодировка:
DISC

OURSE

FUNC

TIONS OF

POSSESS

IVE

PRONO

UNS IN

R

USSIAN

Two main functions of possessive pronouns (PossPrn) in Russian are identifying the possessor ( `my dog') and filling an argument slot of a noun ( `her return'). Sometimes possessive pronouns, especially the reflexive ones, bear additional components of meaning (see [Paducheva 1983: 6-7]). This paper focuses on cases when a PossPrn doesn't have such additional components, and yet the possessor is unambiguously determined by the context. In such cases the PossPrn seems to be redundant:
(1) i i... Lit. Hei squinted hisi strict grey eyes...

One could suppose that such usages of PossPrn are nothing more than just duplication of information, which is a normal property of human languages; however, two observations can be made here. First, possessive markers are well known to be used in discourse functions (for examples from Uralic and Turkic languages see [Fraurud 2001, Nikolaeva 2003]). And second, if we consider a corpus of such "redundant" usages of PossPrns, we may notice that their distribution is not random but rather can be reduced to certain contexts. Two main functions of possessive pronouns can be distinguished in these contexts: (i) maintaining the referential coherence of the text, and (ii) creating of additional focus of attention. One of the marked positions of a possessive pronoun is the position before its antecedent:
(2) [ i] -i ... [ . ] `It is with great difficulties that Shkaf-Petrovich was making his speech...' (Lit. [Speech hisi] Shkaf-Petrovichi pronounced with great difficulties.)

Such examples apparently violate the Principle of referntial compactness as formulated in [Paducheva 2004:355]: "A referentially autonomous NP (in particular, a NP the reference of which is known to the speaker) must be introduced into denotative space of the utterance earlier than its referentially dependent NPs." According to this principle, the referentially dependent noun `speech' should be introduced after the referent which expresses its Agent (- `ShkafPetrovich'). When this principle is being violated, it is the possessive pronoun which indicates to the hearer that the referentially underspecified NP will be specified later on. Most of such usages of PossPrns only seem redundant if we consider contexts as a whole; but as soon as we take into account the unfolding of the discourse in time (i.e. the linear order of elements), they turn out to be the first markers indicating the possessor or filling an argument slot, and are thus ensuring the referential coherence of the text. Another typical context for "redundant" PossPrn are NPs with modifiers. In [Podlesskaya, Rakhilina 1999: 515] reflexive PossPrns in such contexts, being used with body-part nouns, are said to indicate that the body-part deserves a special characteristic. In examples like (1) the reflexive PossPrn has the meaning `inherent to X, characteristic of X'. It serves as an operator which attracts the hearer's attention to the whole NP. On the semantic level, this operator creates a new predication: `X (Subject = Possessor) has strict grey eyes.' As a result, the presupposed status of the possessive relation switches to asserted. The hypothesis that a PossPrn may have different meaning in NPs containig modifiers is partially supported by evidence from other languages, e.g. Kosraean (a Micronesian language), where according to [Fox 1981: 339] different possessive constructions are used depending on whether an NP has any modifiers. 1


References Paducheva 1983 -- . . 1983. . // . . 21. . 3-34. Fox, Barbara. 1981. Body part Syntax: Towards a universal characterization. // Studies in Language. Vol. 5. Pp. 323-342. Fraurud K. 2001. Possessives with extensive use: A source of definite articles? // Baron I., Herslund M., and SЬrensen F. (eds.). Dimensions of Possession. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Pp. 243-268. Nikolaeva, Irina. 2003. Possessive affixes as markers of information structuring: Evidence from Uralic. // Comrie, Bernard, Valery Solovyev, and Pirkko Suihkonen (eds.) International Symposium on the Typology of Argument Structure and Grammatical Relations in Languages Spoken in Europe and North and Central Asia. Udmurt State University, Izhevsk, Udmurt Repaublic, Russia, May 21-24, 2001. Collection of Papers. Udmurt State University, Izhevsk, and Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Pp. 130-145. Paducheva, Elena V. 2004. Splitting of Possessive NPs and External Possessor in Russian. // Ji-Yung Kim, Yury A Lander, Barbara H. Partee (eds.) Possessives and Beyond: Semantics and Syntax. University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 29. Amherts, Mass., USA: GLSA Publications. Pp. 351-363. Podlesskaya, Vera I., Rakhilina, Ekaterina V. 1999. External Possession, Reflexivization and Body Parts in Russian. // Payne D. L., Barshi I. (eds.) 1999. Pp. 505-521.

2